“The Wandering Nerve Linking Heart and Mind” – The Complementary Role of Transcutaneous Vagus Nerve Stimulation in Modulating Neuro-Cardiovascular and Cognitive Performance





The vagus nerve is the longest nerve in the human body, providing afferent information about visceral sensation, integrity and somatic sensations to the CNS via brainstem nuclei to subcortical and cortical structures. Its efferent arm influences GI motility and secretion, cardiac ionotropy, chonotropy and heart rate variability, blood pressure responses, bronchoconstriction and modulates gag and cough responses via palatine and pharyngeal innervation. Vagus nerve stimulation has been utilized as a successful treatment for intractable epilepsy and treatment-resistant depression, and new non-invasive transcutaneous (t-VNS) devices offer equivalent therapeutic potential as invasive devices without the surgical risks. t-VNS offers exciting potential as a therapeutic intervention in cognitive decline and aging populations, classically affected by reduced cerebral perfusion by modulating both limbic and frontal cortical structures, regulating cerebral perfusion and improving parasympathetic modulation of the cardiovascular system. In this narrative review we summarize the research to date investigating the cognitive effects of VNS therapy, and its effects on neurocardiovascular stability.

Keywords: vagus nerve stimulation, cognition, neurocardiovascular control, cerebral blood flow, LC-NE system, inhibitory control, executive function

Introduction

Vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) as a neurostimulation technique and has received renewed attention in recent years. Traditionally invasive VNS (iVNS) devices were sutured under the skin of the chest with a lateral left neck dissection undertaken to expose the left cervical vagus nerve and wrap a stimulating electrode around it. Each iVNS device is costly and up to 30% of patients have side effects post implantation (). Since the development in the early 2000s of peripheral stimulating devices that harness the vagus nerve’s innervation of the skin of the external ear demonstrating efficacy in treating epilepsy, depression and headaches, interest in wider therapeutic potentials of this treatment have grown ().

Declining cognition associated with aging is a burgeoning global health crisis, with at least 152.8 million persons projected to have dementia worldwide by the year 2050 (). There are few effective treatments for cognitive decline and dementia, with no current cure () and although the first disease modifying anti-amyloid agent has been licensed by the FDA (), more therapies are urgently needed to help alleviate the personal, societal and economic cost of increasing dementia diagnoses (). Impaired cognition is associated with impaired autonomic function, specifically impaired parasympathetic measures of heart rate variability (HRV) () likely reflective of the complex interplay between cognition and cardiac modulation, via the central autonomic network.

Studies of patients with intractable epilepsy and treatment -resistant depression treated with iVNS devices showed signals indicating increased alertness and potentially cognitive improvements () and a small pilot study investigated iVNS devices in patients with Alzheimer’s Disease with overall positive results (). Recent meta-analysis of t-VNS in young healthy adults has found an overall moderate effect especially for improved cognitive performance especially executive function (). However the neuroanatomical substrates of persons with treatment-resistant depression or epilepsy are likely both widely variable, and grossly different to both a young cognitively healthy adult and a person with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or dementia and dedicated larger studies are required to investigate if t-VNS has therapeutic potential in this population.

The purpose of this narrative review will be to outline the research to date investigating both cognitive outcomes of VNS in healthy and clinical populations, and the effect VNS has on HRV as a measure of autonomic tone. The mechanisms of action of VNS including neurotransmitter release, local increased cerebral blood flow and modulation of peripheral hemodynamics are discussed and future research recommendations outlined.

Anatomy and Physiology of the Vagus Nerve

The longest nerve in the body, the vagus nerve derives its name from the Latin for ‘straying’ or ‘wandering.’ Aptly named, the nerve has an extensive course, traveling from the medulla to the gut. The vagus nerve’s function is to transmit information to and from the central nervous system (CNS) regarding control of the gastrointestinal, cardiovascular, and respiratory systems. It is comprised of approximately 80% afferent and 20% efferent fibers () including A, B and C fibers classified by conduction velocity (). Vagus neurons may involve visceral (cardiac, bronchopulmonary, gastrointestinal) or somatic (soft tissues, muscles of palate, pharynx) modulation. Afferent fibers are further sub classified as general visceral afferent, general somatic afferent, or special visceral afferent. Two efferent fiber types are recognized, namely special visceral efferent and general visceral efferent (see Table 1). Fibers connect centrally to four vagal nuclei; the nucleus of the solitary tract (NTS) and spinal trigeminal nucleus which contain vagal afferent fibers and the nucleus ambiguous and dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus (DMN) from where vagal efferent fibers leave ().

TABLE 1.

The constituent fibers of the vagus nerve.

FIBER
BC
Fiber diameter13–20 mm6–12 mm1–5 mm1–5 mm0.4–2 mm
Gross anatomical structureLargeLargeLargeSmallSmall
Main function afferentSomatic touch pain temperatureSomatic touchVisceral: pain stretch chemical, temperatureVisceralVisceral: pain stretch chemical, temperature
Main function efferentMuscle toneMuscle preganglionicpreganglionicpreganglionicpreganglionic
Myelin++++
Threshold mA0.02–0.2 mA0.02–0.2 mA0.02–0.2 mA0.04–0.6 mA0.3–6 mA
Conduction velocity ms8–120 ms35–75 ms3–30 ms3–15 ms0.5–2 ms
Purported effect of VNS on EEGSynchronizationSynchronizationSynchronizationSynchronizationDesynchronization

Adapted from .

Afferent vagus fibers enter the medulla at the level of the olive, and terminate primarily in the NTS (). Each vagus nerve (VN) synapses bilaterally in the NTS; so vagal afferent information is processed bilaterally in the CNS (). Second order afferent fibers from the NTS project most densely to the parabrachial nucleus of the pons (PBN) with the NTS also projecting to noradrenergic (locus coeruleus) and serotonergic (raphe nuclei) neuromodulatory systems (). From here vagal information is relayed to a number of mostly subcortical structures, including the hypothalamus, the central nucleus of the amygdala, the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, and the intralaminar thalamic nucleus. Vagal afferent information is also sent to the anterior insular cortex which communicates with more rostral regions of the cortex (orbital and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex) and also indirectly with the medial prefrontal cortex ().

These central structures are part of the central autonomic network (CAN) which is thought to be the origin of autonomic, behavioral, cognitive, and endocrine responses, capable of modulating the functioning of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) via descending pathways projecting onto sympathetic pre-ganglionic neurons in the spinal cord and onto the DMN at the origin of vagal efferents (). The central connections of the DMN are considerable, with afferent projections arising from sites including the NST, magnocellular paraventricular nuclei and several medullary nuclei (). Whilst a minority of efferent fibers connect centrally, most DMN fibers project to GI organs via parasympathetic ganglia located close to or in the walls of viscera. Further efferent fibers originate from the nucleus ambiguous (NA), a motor nucleus located in the reticular formation of the medulla which gives rise to preganglionic neurons innervating the heart and lungs () which exert a cardio-inhibitory effect mediated via the sinoatrial and atrioventricular ganglia (). The right vagus nerve mostly innervates the sinoatrial node (involved in the pacemaker function of the heart) whereas the left vagus is mostly thought to innervate the atrioventricular node (regulating the force of contraction of the cardiac myocytes with less influence over heart rate) however comprehensive human studies confirming this precise delineation are needed (). The dorsal branchiomotor division of the NA is the site of origin of efferent fibers innervating striated muscle of the palate, pharynx, larynx and upper esophagus.

See Figure 1 for a schematic representation of the VN fibers and central projections.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Schematic representation of afferent and efferent fibers of the vagus nerve and central projections.

History of Vagus Nerve Stimulation

Vagus nerve stimulation was initially proposed as a therapeutic intervention in 1871 () and a device was designed to stimulate bilateral vagus nerves in the late 19th century (). Preclinical studies in the 1930–1950s demonstrated via EEG signaling that VNS had cortical stimulating activity (), and could terminate canine seizures ().

Invasive VNS (iVNS) received United States regulatory approval for the adjunctive treatment of refractory seizures in 1997 and for use in treatment resistant depression in 2005 (). However, given the invasive nature of iVNS (requiring general anesthesia, thoracic implantation of a battery generator, and neck dissection to attach stimulating electrodes to the left cervical vagus nerve), the concept of non-invasive VNS was proposed in 2000 whereby, drawing on evidence from studies of auricular acupuncture, it was postulated that transcutaneous vagal stimulation could represent a valuable tool in epilepsy treatment (). Non-invasive VNS involves using stimulating electrodes on the skin to excite afferent vagal fibers and can be performed via the ear (transcutaneous auricular VNS: t-VNS) or the neck (transcutaneous cervical VNS: tcVNS). For the purposes of this narrative review non-invasive VNS will refer to auricular t-VNS.

The technique of t-VNS exploits the peripheral anatomy of the vagus nerve, activating vagal afferent projections through stimulation of the auricular branch of vagus nerve (ABVN) at the ear () see Figure 2 for a schematic representation of the anatomy of the ABVN and central structures it modulates. Anti-seizure efficacy equivalent to iVNS was demonstrated in preclinical studies before the feasibility and therapeutic significance of this technique in humans were demonstrated () and evidence from multiple functional brain imaging studies confirms significant activation of central vagal projections via this non-invasive method ().

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Schematic diagram of innervation of ABVN and central projections, adapted with permission from .

Transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation waveforms can be delivered at a variety of different parameter settings which vary frequency (Hz), amplitude (mA), pulse width (μs-msec) and duration of stimulation. It is currently being investigated as a therapeutic intervention for a variety of medical disorders including epilepsy, migraine and cluster headaches, tinnitus, atrial fibrillation, Parkinson’s disease, schizophrenia, impaired glucose tolerance, obesity, and pain (). There is particular interest in the evolving literature reporting the use of t-VNS in cognitive disorders (). Potential mechanisms of action include modulation of HRV, impacts on cerebral perfusion, and noradrenergic neuromodulation. The complementary role of vagus nerve stimulation in modulating neuro-cardiovascular and cognitive performance is explored in detail below.

See Figure 2 for a schematic diagram of the area of innervation of the ABVN and its central projections.

Cognitive Performance and Vagus Nerve Stimulation

Brain imaging during t-VNS demonstrates strong activation of vagal projections to subcortical nuclei and frontal brain regions, i.e., superior frontal gyrus and medial frontal gyrus during stimulation () (See below in “Mechanisms of Action” for further detailed discussion regarding the neuroanatomical structures modulated during VNS). Cognitive effects of both iVNS and t-VNS in both clinical populations and healthy volunteers will be examined under the following themes: Cognitive control, i.e., the non-automatic regulation of behavior to achieve a goal () a primarily executive function that involves suppression of goal-irrelevant stimuli via response and attention-inhibition () and it primarily involves the lateral prefrontal cortex (); Language, both assessing categorical fluency a semantic memory language task involving the temporal lobe, and word recognition and retrieval which mostly involves episodic working memory, involving prefrontal cortex and medial temporal structures (); Associative memory, a subcategory of declarative episodic memory and involves the ability to link disparate novel stimuli (); Emotion recognition as a subtype of cognition involves areas of the brain involved in perceiving social information including the medial prefrontal cortex and the orbitofrontal cortex () and regions implicated in emotional processing, including the cortical orbitofrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate cortex but also subcortical structures including the amygdala, hypothalamus, basal ganglia and the periaqueductal gray matter ().

Interest in the potential role of VNS as a cognitive enhancer started following a preclinical rodent study of an inhibitory-avoidance task. Subjects received a single exposure to a foot shock followed immediately by VNS or sham. Those undergoing true VNS stimulation had longer step times demonstrating enhanced avoidance and this effect was modulated by the intensity of the stimulus, with 0.4 mA being an effective level of stimulation and 0.2 and 0.8 mA having no significant effect (). Subsequent in-human trials tested word recognition in patients with intractable epilepsy who had iVNS devices implanted 2–24 weeks prior to testing. The stimulation parameters were 30Hz, 0.5 mA at 0.5 ms pulse width compared to an amplitude of 0.75–1 mA, and improved word recognition was only found in the group stimulated at the lower amplitude (). These results paved the way for further investigation in this area as detailed below.

Vagus Nerve Stimulation and Cognitive Control, i.e., Executive Function in Healthy Volunteers

Inhibitory control is commonly measured using performance on tasks such as the Stroop, Eriksen Flanker (Flanker), and Simon tasks, i.e., forced-choice reaction time tasks that require participants to selectively attend and respond to target stimuli whilst ignoring goal-irrelevant distracting stimuli ().

Enhanced response times, as reflected by participants’ ability to stop a process and change to another response simultaneously and sequentially, and increased post error slowing were demonstrated during t-VNS (). Post error slowing refers to appropriate slowing after negative feedback or unforeseen errors and is linked to the activity of the locus coeruleus–norepinephrine (LC–NE) system and therefore postulated to be enhanced by VNS. As with the above trials, there were fewer false alarms during a more challenging paradigm with t-VNS when working memory processes were simultaneously engaged () and improved response selection and control performance was demonstrated with t-VNS in a serial reaction time test in young volunteers (). In a sequence learning paradigm, the presentation of so-called reversal trials is associated with longer response latencies as compared to non-reversal trials, a result attributable to the ‘inhibition of return’ type phenomenon. Inhibition of return refers to an inhibitory after-effect of attention whereby, following exogenous orientation of attention to a stimulus, processing of stimuli at this location is first facilitated and then inhibited ().  demonstrated that active t-VNS, as compared to sham stimulation in the context of a serial reaction time test, reduced reaction time for reversal trials, eliminating the inhibition of return like effect described above.

In a similar experimental set up, increased attention, globally enhanced accuracy and reduced performance costs were demonstrated in a Stop-Change paradigm with t-VNS ().

Results in this area have not been uniformly positive. In a testing paradigm in healthy volunteers using higher than average amplitude settings (see Table 2) there were no improvements in a Stroop test, Modified Flanker test or a number/letter working memory task with t-VNS. Improved accuracy in a dimensional change card sorting task was however noted (). Similar previous studies failed to show improved behavioral performance with t-VNS () however non-performance parameters, namely a frontal EEG signal (P3 amplitude) thought to change with response inhibition and higher salivary amylase levels, were noted in the intervention group (). Further studies investigating EEG amplitudes affected by t-VNS and cognitive control paradigms included one involving an acoustic rather than visual oddball paradigm. In this context, t-VNS augmented the P3 amplitude, and with random noise stimulation with t-VNS reaction times were reduced (). There are myriad potential reasons for replication challenges in this newly expanding area of research and may include stimulation parameter differences including lack of pre-testing active stimulation.

TABLE 2.

Cognition and VNS in healthy volunteer populations.

COGNITION AND VNS: Healthy volunteers
Stimulation Parameters
StudyiVNS/tVNSHzmAPulse widthTimePopulationTaskOutcome
tVNS25Hz0.5mA200–300 μs30 s blocksHealthy young adult volunteers
n = 30
Stop change paradigmEnhanced response selection and faster response times when two actions executed in succession
tVNS left outer auditory canal25Hz0.5 mA200–300 μs30 s blocksHealthy young adult volunteers
n = 40
Modified Flanker testIncreased post error slowing during active tVNS
tVNS left external acoustic meatus8Hz5.0 mA200 μs17 minHealthy older adults avg age 60.5
n = 30
-Face name recognition task
−15 word learning test
-Digit span forward/backward
-Verbal fluency test
-Concept shifting task
-Letter digit subtraction
-Stroop color word test
Higher number of accurate “hits” during tVNS for face name recognition
tVNS left inner ear25Hz0.5 mA200–300 μs30 s on/30 s offHealthy young volunteers n = 51Inhibitory control (go-no-go task)Fewer false alarms in the more challenging paradigm, i.e., when working memory processes also engaged
tVNS
Left outer auditory canal
25Hz0.5 mA200–300 μs30 s on/30 s offHealthy young volunteers n = 38Emotion recognition Reading the mind in the Eyes testEnhanced emotion recognition for easy (not challenging) items suggesting it promoted the ability to decode salient social cues
tVNS25HzAvg 1.3 mA (0.4–3.3)200–300 μsContinuousHealthy adult volunteers
n = 21
-Adapted response conflict Simon task
-Novelty oddball task
No behavioral change noted
Down-regulated N2 potential EEG reading
tVNS
Left cymba conchae
25 Hz−1.3 mA (0.4–3.3) active
−1.49 mA (0.6–4.8) sham
200–300 μs28 min task 1
7 min task 2
Healthy young volunteers n = 21-Novelty oddball task
-number version of the Simon task
-No difference with tVNS with difficult targets or novel stimuli
-Difference between tVNS and sham stimulation (P3 amplitude) in EEG parameters for easy targets associated with larger increase in sAA levels after tVNS
tVNS
Left cymba conchae
25Hz0.5 mA250 μs30 s on/30 s off
Started 90 min prior to task
Healthy young volunteers n = 20 avg age 24.8-Acoustic oddball paradigm (respond as quickly as possible whenever a target tone was detected)- tVNS increased EEG parameter P3 amplitude
- Random noise stimulation reduced the reaction time
tVNS at left ear both anterior (cymba conchae) and posterior of ear−80Hz
−10Hz
-No stim
10–15 mA180 μs in square waveform25–35 min lead in timeHealthy adult males n = 18Two olfactory tests (odor threshold test (OTT) and supra-threshold test (STT)High frequency (80Hz) VNS positively modulated olfactory performance in healthy participants and showed significant increase in NIRS recordings of the right hemispheric orbitofrontal cortex
tVNS left concha n = 40
sham left earlobe n = 40
25Hz0.5 mA200–300 μs15 min lead in timeHealthy young volunteers n = 80 (50 females, 30 males, mean age 20.96)Convergent and divergent thinking tasks-Fluency scores were significantly higher in the active tVNS group (able to generate more answers)
-tVNS affected cognitive flexibility, i.e., participants could think of more different categories than sham
tVNS left medial acoustic meatus25Hz0.5 mA200–300 μs30 s blocks
15 min lead in time
Healthy young adult volunteers
n = 40
Serial reaction time testEnhanced response selection process and action control performance
tVNS
Left cymba conchae
25Hz0.5 mA250 μs30 s on/30 s off
10 min lead in time
Healthy young volunteers n = 61Computerized fear conditioning, fear generalization, and fear extinction paradigmNo difference in physiological and declarative indices of fear between tVNS and sham conditions
tVNS cymba conchae25Hz0.5 mA for 16
0.54–0.57 mA for rest
250 μs30 s during consolidationHealthy volunteers
n = 41 age avg 22.2
n = 24 age avg 55.1
Word recognition taskNo effect on verbal word memory
tVNS left cymba conchae (active) or left earlobe (sham)25HzActive 1.48 mA ± 0.59 sham 1.31 mA ± 0.5200–300 μs30 s on/30 s off
Stimulated for 23 min 5 min before 13 min during and 5 in after lexical decision task
Healthy volunteers
n = 60
−46 = female
-avg age 23.45
Lexical decision task and recognition memory task of selected German words (either emotionally charged or neutral)
Also – BP, HR and sAA
Overall no effect of tVNS on task performance or word recognition memory – however higher recollection based memory performance was observed during tVNS than sham
tVNS25Hz2.19 mA (±0.93)200–300 μs30 s on/30 s off
4 min lead in time
Healthy adult volunteers n = 35-Modified Flanker test
-Spatial Stroop task
-Number/Letter task
-Dimensional change card sorting task
Only the DCCS shows improvement with tVNS
tVNS left cymba conchae25Hz2.37 mA (±0.16)200 μs30 s on/30 s off
30 min lead in time
Healthy adult volunteers n = 22Stop Change paradigm (go-no-go task)Globally enhanced accuracy across conditions
-Reduced the performance costs of go/change response conflicts
-increased attention

Study 1
tVNS
Left cymba conchae
Sham: no stimulation
25HzOnline 0.7 ± 0.36 mA
Offline 0.69 ± 0.38 mA
Sham 0.73 ± 0.27 mA
500 μs30 s on and 30 s off
25 min pre task (offline) or 15 min during task (online)
Healthy young volunteer n = 46 (25 female, average age 20.39 ± 1.96)Spatial stimuli task
Four blocks with 72 experiment trials in each block
Offline (pre-task stim for 25 min) tVNS significantly increased hits in spatial 3-back task but not rejections or reaction times

Study 2
tVNS
left cymba conchae
sham; active stimulation of earlobe
25 HzActive:
0.74 mA ± 0.37
Sham: 0.84 mA ± 0.39
500 μs30 s on and 30 s off
Both 25 min stimulation
Healthy young volunteers n = 58 (24 female, average age 19.9 ± 1.49)Spatial stimuli task
Four blocks with 72 experiment trials in each block
Offline (pre- task stimulation for 25 min) tVNS improved hits but not correct rejections or reaction time of accurate trials in spatial WM performance
tVNS
Left tragus
25Hz1–6 mA250 μsHealthy young volunteers n = 33 control n = 29 experiment-Word retention:
– non-rhyming, easily separable words
-rhyming words
tVNS was associated with higher accuracy but only when the items are phonologically similar
Cervical VNS via gammaCore device
cVNS vs. sham (n = 20 both groups)
25HzNot availableNot available2 min cyclesHealthy young military recruits n = 40 (M:F 33:7) avg age 28 ± 6 years34 h of continuous sleep deprivation
Air Force–Multi-Attribute Task Battery (AF-MATB);
simultaneously monitor
and respond to four separate cognitive process tasks: a visual system alert monitoring task, a visual–motor tracking task, an auditory communication monitoring task and a management
task
cVNS significantly improved objective arousal and multitasking
for as long as 24-h post-stimulation
Subjective ratings of
fatigue also improved

The most recent studies in this area have involved a spatial stimulation and response inhibition multitask, with notable improved results in accuracy with 25 min pre-assessment t-VNS stimulation () and improved objective attention, arousal and multitasking ability in sleep deprived military personnel ().

Vagus Nerve Stimulation and Language in Healthy Volunteers

Fluency scores in healthy volunteers during a convergent and divergent thinking task were significantly higher during active t-VNS at the left conchae, and categorical flexibility (i.e., participants’ ability to think of more and varied categories of nouns) was also significantly improved (). However, an experimental design investigating the difference in effect of t-VNS on word recognition memory in young compared to older volunteers (average age 22.2 and 55.1) whereby t-VNS was delivered for 30 s during the consolidation phase of a word recognition memory task showed no improvement in accuracy scores for immediate recall or delayed recognition in both age groups (). Possible reasons for this may be that 30 s of t-VNS may be insufficient for a non-invasive device to effectively stimulate the vagal afferent pathway, that longer and more repetitive stimulation of the vagus nerve might be required to effectively modulate hippocampal processes via synaptic plasticity. A recent investigation of word retention, stimulating the left tragus with t-VNS at again similar parameters but wider amplitude found improved accuracy in word retention but only in items that rhymed, i.e., were phonologically similar ().

Vagus Nerve Stimulation and Associative Memory in Healthy Volunteers

Transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation has been tested in a group of healthy older adults to determine the technique’s impact on performance in a face-name association task (). VNS was employed in the encoding and consolidation phases of the task with active and sham stimulation compared in a randomized crossover design. Active t-VNS was demonstrated to increase the number of ‘hits’ on the memory task. Stimulation parameters employed differed somewhat from those seen in the broader literature concerning the impact of VNS on cognitive function. A stimulation intensity of 5.0 mA, a pulse width of 0.2 ms, and a frequency of 8Hz were utilized, citing previous functional and electrophysiological studies (). A stimulation lead in time of 17 min was also utilized, which has been theorized to be beneficial for targeted neuronal plasticity ().

Vagus Nerve Stimulation and Emotion Recognition in Healthy Volunteers

This ability to recognize different emotions in others was investigated and found to be enhanced by t-VNS at the left outer auditory canal in young healthy adults but only for objectively easy, not challenging, items via the Reading the Mind in the Eyes test (). Subsequent investigations of fear conditioning and extinction in young volunteers, after previous positive studies, found that t-VNS at the left cymba conchae did not infer any difference in physiological or declarative indices of fear or improve fear extinction (). Further studies are needed in this area to elucidate if t-VNS has a specific beneficial effect, given its ability to modulate both cortical and subcortical structures.

See Table 2 for parameters settings and outcomes in trials of VNS in healthy volunteers.

Vagus Nerve Stimulation and Cognition in Clinical Populations

In this section we highlight the studies to date investigating the cognitive effects of VNS on clinical populations, mostly with treatment-resistant depression or epilepsy. Many studies investigating the role of VNS in clinical populations has involved invasive VNS (iVNS). A further potential confounder is the impact some of these underlying pathologies have on cognition, the altered medial temporal anatomy especially in cases of epilepsy and the medications used to manage these conditions can also have deleterious effects on cognition.

Vagus Nerve Stimulation and Cognitive Control, i.e., Executive Function in Clinical Populations

Vagus nerve stimulation has been shown experimentally to have mixed results when examining the subdomain of decision making, specifically on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT). In one paradigm eleven patients with refractory epilepsy and iVNS devices completed a gambling task involving control and experimental trials with active VNS synchronized to stimulate in the latter. Whilst improved performance was demonstrated in the earlier part of the task, this trend was reversed later in the experimental trial with active stimulation trending toward being detrimental to performance (). Technical failure and a cumulative stimulation-dose effect were amongst the potential explanations proposed by the authors to explain this phenomenon. Decision-making may depend on intact working memory () and several studies have demonstrated working memory involvement in the IGT () which may have affected results in this study.

Working memory refers to a cognitive process that provides temporary storage and manipulation of the information necessary for complex cognitive tasks (). Literature concerning the impact of acutely administered VNS on working memory is promising but limited to a small number of studies. In one experimental paradigm, twenty participants with poorly controlled epilepsy were required to perform a computer-based Executive-Reaction Time (Executive RT) Test, wherein ability to memorize and store the orientation of a triangle and indicate its position in response to a go signal were assessed whilst VNS was delivered in a cyclic fashion. Active iVNS stimulation was associated with fewer errors in the subtask relying on working memory ().

The effect of active iVNS on response inhibition was also assessed by employing a classic stop-signal task in participants with refractory epilepsy (). Quicker response inhibition has been demonstrated during active stimulation in patients who had previously shown a larger therapeutic effect of VNS. The beneficial effects of VNS on cognitive control may be maximally demonstrated in so-called ‘VNS responders’ (for the primary clinical indication) as demonstrated by patients with iVNS devices who undertook the Eriksen Flanker task during both VNS ‘on’ and ‘off’ stimulation. Only those deemed VNS responders (i.e., those whose seizure frequency had decreased by >50% post-device implantation) had demonstrable improved reaction times and reduced distractor interference during active stimulation (). There is a subcategory of patients with refractory epilepsy who do not respond to iVNS therapy, i.e., do not have seizure reduction of 50%, and deemed “non-responders.” It is notable that a current output of 2.28 mA was utilized in the VNS “responder” group and it’s possible that, in keeping with previous studies examining optimal amplitude for stimulation, that the higher amplitudes employed exceeded that at which cognitive control is optimized for the iVNS “non-responders.” Further research is needed in this area in particular regarding stimulation parameters and iVNS responders.

Vagus Nerve Stimulation and Language in Clinical Populations

In the first study of its kind, building on previous preclinical research, the impact of iVNS on word retrieval memory was assessed via an experimental protocol whereby participants with iVNS devices inserted for epilepsy control, were required to read a series of paragraphs, and subsequently identify words that were highlighted in the text. The study population comprised two groups of patients who were administered active (0.5–1.5 mA) or sham VNS, delivered 2-min after learning in the memory consolidation phase. An inverted U-shaped relationship was demonstrated regarding stimulus intensity and modulation of cognitive performance, with memory enhancing effects demonstrated only at moderate intensities, namely 0.5 mA (). These results were in part corroborated by a subsequent study which employed higher stimulation intensities (>1.0 mA) and failed to demonstrate enhancement of verbal recognition memory, in fact demonstrating a reversible deterioration in figural memory (). However, study design may have impacted cognitive outcomes here as delivery of stimulation was not restricted to the consolidation period. The propensity for iVNS to positively impact word retrieval memory in a population of patients being treated with iVNS for intractable epilepsy was highlighted again in 2006 whereby the impact of iVNS on performance in the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test was assessed, demonstrating a significant improvement in word retention when active (amplitude 0.5 mA) as opposed to sham stimulation was applied during memory consolidation ().

Vagus Nerve Stimulation and Emotional Recognition in Clinical Populations

The effect of t-VNS on participants’ ability to recognize facial emotions in three experimental paradigms (graded presentation, static images and in a go-no-go task) was assessed in a group of adolescents diagnosed with major depressive disorder (MDD). In non-depressed controls t-VNS delivered at 1Hz, 0.5 mA 30 s block with 15 min lead in time, demonstrated enhanced recognition of emotions but notably led to a significant decrease in the ability of those with MDD to recognize sad emotions ().

See Table 3 for parameters settings and outcomes in trials of VNS in clinical populations and please see below “VNS, cognition and HRV” for a discussion of VNS in Alzheimer’s disease.

TABLE 3.

VNS and cognition in clinical populations.

COGNITION AND VNS: Clinical Populations
Stimulation Parameters
StudyiVNS/tVNSHzmAPulse widthTimePopulationTaskOutcome
iVNS
2–24/52 post implantation
30Hz−0.5 mA
−0.75–1.5 mA
0.5 ms30 sIntractable
epilepsy
n = 10
Word recognition taskImproved word recognition memory only when 0.5 mA delivered post reading
iVNS
Assessed at 3 and 6 months
20Hz0.25 mA, increased 0.25 mA increments over 2 weeks then fixed500 μs30 s followed by 5 min pauseProbable Alzheimer’s
n = 10 age 67 ± 7.6 8 women 2 men
Median change in ADAS-cog
Median change in MMSE after 3 and 6/12
Depression, behavior and QOL variables
After 6/12 8 of 10 patients showed improvement from 3/12 ADAS-cog scores
After 6/12 7 of 10 patients improved MMSE score by average 2.5 points
No change in other variables
iVNS30Hz0.5 mA500 μs60 sIntractable epilepsy
n = 11
Iowa Gambling TaskConflicting results, deleterious at higher doses
iVNS
At least 1 year of VNS treatment
20Hz0.25 mA, increased in 0.25 mA increments over 2 weeks then fixed500 μs30 s followed by 5 min pauseProbable Alzheimer’s
n = 17 (age 63 range 57–81)
11 women 6 men
Median change in ADAS-cog
Median change in MMSE after 1 year
Depression, behavior and QOL variables
At 1 year, 41% had improvement or no decline from baseline on ADAS-cog
70% had improvement or no decline on MMSE
No change in other variables
iVNS
5–7/12 post implantation
30HzMean 1.75 mA (range 1–2.5)500 μs30 s–4.5 minIntractable
Epilepsy
n = 11
Word recognition task
Design recognition task
Deterioration in figural recognition memory
iVNS
12–16/52 after implantation
30 Hz in high stim group
1 Hz in low stim group
Avg 1.3 mA in high simulation group
Avg 1.2 mA in low stimulation group
500 μs
130 μs
30 s on every 5 min
30 s on every 3 h
Intractable
Epilepsy
n = 160
Wonderlic personell test, Stroop test, Digit cancelation, Symbol Digit ModalitiesNo significant changes were noted in the cognitive tests in low or high stimulation
iVNS
>3/12 post implantation
X0.5 mAx30 sIntractable
Epilepsy
n = 10
Hopkins verbal learning testImproved retention index
iVNS
12/12 post implantation
30 Hz0.5–3 mA avg 1.72 ± 0.53500 μs30 s every 5 minIntractable epilepsy n = 16Memory Observation QuestionnaireImproved subjective and objective memory scores compared to baseline, but similar to medical management
iVNS
>18/12 post implantation
Avg 25 (20–30)Avg 2.3 mA (0.75–3.0)Avg 431 μs (130–500 μs)7 s on/
18 s off
Intractable epilepsy
n = 20
Stop signal taskVNS responders demonstrated quicker response inhibition
iVNS 2–130 months post implantation30Hz1.5–1.75 mA250 μs30 s on/48 s offIntractable epilepsy
n = 20
Executive reaction time test (go-no-go task)Improved working memory (only when 3 participants with cognitive impairment removed)
iVNS20 or 30 HzAvg 2.28 mA
(0.75–3.0)
250 μs or 500 μs7 s on/
18 s off
Intractable epilepsy
n = 17
Eriksen Flanker taskVNS responders demonstrated improved reaction times and decreased distraction interference
tVNS
Left conchae
1Hz0.5 mA250 μs30 s on/30 s off
15 min lead in time
-Adolescents with major depressive disorder
n = 33
control group: adolescents with headache n = 30
Facial emotional recognition in three tests
1. As a graded presentation
2. As static images
3. in a go – no -go task
-In non-depressed controls tVNS enhances the general ability to recognize emotions
-tVNS specifically led to a decrease in the recognition of sad emotions in patients with MDD

Linking Brain and Heart: Potential Mechanisms of Action of Vagus Nerve Stimulation-Mediated Cognitive Enhancement

There are many potential mechanisms through which VNS may exert its cognitive enhancing effects, including direct neurotransmitter release, increased cerebral perfusion to discreet neuroanatomical structures, reduced neuro-inflammation and via modulation of peripheral hemodynamics. For the purposes of this narrative review, we will analyze the link between cerebral blood flow, cerebral autoregulation and cardiac modulation. Beyond the scope of this review is how t-VNS may therapeutically affect the inflammatory cascade via activating the cholinergic anti-inflammatory pathway and the beneficial effects this may have in aging populations.

Mechanism of Action: Vagus Nerve Stimulation and Local Neurotransmitter Release

The main neurotransmitters centrally released via the afferent projections of the vagus nerve are thought to be GABA and Norepinephrine (NE). For a comprehensive review of the preclinical and clinical studies detailing the evidence supporting the modulation of these neurotransmitters during iVNS and t-VNS see ().

As the primary inhibitory neurotransmitter in the brain, higher levels of GABA decrease cortical excitability, and is the accepted proposed method for VNS’ anti-seizure efficacy. It has been suggested that increased cortical inhibition due to high GABA levels can sharpen task-relevant representations in the cortex and inhibit competing responses, thereby facilitating response selection and inhibition processes ().

Norepinephrine is a crucial neurotransmitter modulating arousal and attention, and is primarily released via the locus coeruleus (LC). There are two distinct modes of LC firing that are associated with equally distinct modes of attentional strategy. Connections with the orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex are thought to drive the LC-NE system into one of these two stable states of activity, a high tonic (sustained) mode or a phasic (bursting) mode accompanied by moderate tonic activity (). This switching of attentional state via tonic LC activity is thought to result in a flexible attentional system that allows cycling between behaviors to find and meet task demands in one’s environment, i.e., the adaptive gain theory ().

Interestingly, and similar to the effects noted with iVNS stimulation levels and responses by , moderate levels of NE augment prefrontal cortex function, whereas high and low concentrations of NE impair function, i.e., NE exhibits an inverted-U relationship between LC-NE activity and optimal performance on attention tasks (). However, in general as NE levels rise executive function improves, likely via enhanced activation of the prefrontal cortex and frontoparietal control network (). Inhibitory control for action cancelation is specifically enhanced with noradrenergic modulation, likely via this prefrontal cortical network ().

Older adults with more dense LC innervation (i.e., higher neuromelanin MRI contrast) had overall better performance on a reversal memory tasks () and had improved cognitive reserve (). Similarly in a post-mortem study of patients with Alzheimer’s disease, lower LC cell integrity and greater cortical tangle density was associated with greater tau burden beyond the medial temporal lobes and worsening memory decline, identifying LC integrity as a promising indicator of initial AD-related processes ().

Studies have also demonstrated a decline in GABA concentration in frontal and parietal regions in aging populations, areas crucial for cognitive control (). NE and GABA may in fact work synergistically to facilitate executive functioning; GABA by encouraging response inhibition of task irrelevant stimuli and NE via the LC-NE system increasing frontal NE release and thus executive functioning ().

Mechanism of Action: Vagus Nerve Stimulation Increases Cerebral Perfusion

Cerebral autoregulation is the phenomenon by which the brain receives the same cerebral blood flow (CBF) despite variations in perfusion pressure. The aim of autoregulation is to protect the brain against hypoxia and edema as a result of decreased or critically high arterial blood pressures respectively. Multiple factors physiologically modify autoregulation including blood CO2 levels, hypoxia etc. While still controversial, the ANS may play a prominent role in cerebral autoregulation in response to such stimuli, inducing vasodilation or constriction, and parasympathetic and sympathetic nerves are anatomically located in the same perineural sheath innervating cerebral arteries (). The means by which VNS exerts its cognitive enhancing effect is probably multimodal, however modulating CBF is likely a crucial factor.

Multiple modalities have been utilized to assess for CBF changes due to vagus nerve stimulation, including position emission tomography (PET), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) studies and trials of patients with iVNS treatment for epilepsy and depression have demonstrated a variety of CBF modulatory effects at specific cortical and subcortical areas. Increased CBF at the orbitofrontal cortex (), temporal lobe (), insular cortex (), bilateral frontal lobes (), left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex () and subcortical structures including thalamus, hypothalamus, basal ganglia and other nuclei () has been observed. For a comprehensive review see .

Notably analysis undertaken during acute iVNS has noted bilateral decreased hippocampal CBF (). This has been replicated in t-VNS functional imaging studies which have confirmed stimulation and increased CBF at vagally innervated brain regions during auricular t-VNS and notably decreased perfusion at hippocampal regions (). T-VNS has also demonstrated efficacy in increasing arousal in comatose patients who respond to auditory signaling and again the brain regions noted on fMRI to be activated were similar to previous iVNS studies, including left superior temporal gyrus, left prefrontal cortex, left insular cortex, left middle frontal gyrus among other cortical and subcortical structures ().

It is worth considering that intermittently stimulating neurons at different frequencies produces drastically different changes in neuronal behavior with low frequency stimulation inducing long term depression (LTD) and less connectivity while intermittent high frequency stimulation produces long term potentiation (LTP) and increased signaling (). Therefore acute VNS stimulates brain regions mostly involved in alertness and frontal processing, whereas chronic stimulation may improve LTP in classic memory-associated regions, including the hippocampus. Evidence for this can be seen in preclinical studies () but also significant increases in hippocampal gray matter volume over time has been observed in patients with iVNS devices inserted for treatment-resistant depression (). More recently, Near Infrared Spectroscopy (NIRS) has been utilized to monitor cerebral blood flow and increased frontal perfusion in patients with epilepsy was noted during iVNS when paired with a cognitive task ().

Both dementia and even its prodromal stage, MCI, are characterized by a reduction in cerebral blood flow (). A meta-analysis of twenty-six studies investigating CBF in MCI found overall reduced tissue oxygenation, CBF and velocity in MCI compared to healthy controls () and studies are underway investigating the CBF changes that may occur with cognitive stimulation in MCI and dementia (). Similar findings have been noted in patients with Alzheimer’s disease, with reduced CBF in many cortical regions including temporal () parietal () and other regions including precuneus, frontal and posterior cingulate cortex ().

Mechanism of Action: Vagus Nerve Stimulation Modulates Peripheral Hemodynamics

As well as modulating central neurotransmitter release and cerebral blood flow, VNS has been shown to have positive peripheral modulatory effects in pathological states characterized by impaired autonomic regulation including postural orthostatic tachycardia syndrome (POTS) () specifically patients with POTS and impaired vagal cardiac control, as defined by reduced HRV (). T-VNS has also shown benefits in modulating blood pressure in induced orthostatic hypotension (). These studies suggest VNS may have a role in positively manipulating the peripheral baroreceptor-reflex and thus cerebral autoregulation, and potentially may improve cortical perfusion via this route, however further dedicated studies are required to precisely delineate this relationship.

Vagus Nerve Stimulation and Heart Rate Variability

Heart rate variability analysis can be performed via a variety of approaches and is based on the extrapolation of time intervals between each R wave peak (), discounting any ectopic beats or arrhythmias, e.g., atrial fibrillation. The most commonly applied methods to determine HRV are time-domain analysis and frequency/spectral analysis. Indices deriving from the time domain analysis quantify the amount of variance in the selected inter-beat interval employing statistical measures, such as the standard deviation of the normal beat intervals (SDNN) and the root mean square of successive differences between normal beats (RMSSD) (). The spectral analysis of HRV identifies oscillatory rhythms that occur in specific frequency ranges. Three main components of the spectrums can be identified as: the very low frequency band (VLF), below 0.04 Hz, likely influenced by thermoregulatory mechanisms and circadian rhythms; the low-frequency band (LF) between 0.04 and 0.15 Hz in humans, a marker influenced by baroreflex () sympathetic and parasympathetic modulation; the high-frequency band (HF) in the range from 0.15 to 0.4 Hz, a marker of vagal modulation that is influenced by respiratory activity (). One of the limitations of HRV analysis is high within and between individual variability, which may be reduced by longer measurement intervals, i.e., 24 h but which is resultantly harder to process. For a comprehensive review on the various indices please see .

The ANS influences cardiac beat-to-beat interval length in response to several factors. The sympathetic and parasympathetic systems are the principal rapidly reacting systems that control heart rate. The two systems have different latency periods with sympathetic effects on heart rate slower than parasympathetic () i.e., the parasympathetic system has the ability to alter heart rate within 1–2 beats, while sympathetic effects take up to 10 s to take effect.

Low HRV has been associated with poorer prognosis in cardiovascular diseases, cancer, Metabolic Syndrome and Alzheimer’s disease and it has been postulated that related pathophysiological mechanisms often contribute to their occurrence and progression, namely inflammatory responses, sympathetic overactivity, and oxidative stress (). Lower vagal nerve activity has been found to be significantly correlated with oxidative stress (), with inflammatory markers in healthy individuals as well as in those with cardiovascular diseases () and anxiety disorders have also been characterized by low HRV (). Experimental studies have long demonstrated the success of behavioral () and pharmacological () interventions in manipulating HRV. Increases in HRV seen with physical fitness training are associated with improvements in executive function (). The links between executive function and cardiac autonomic regulation were further highlighted by a recent study examining the impact of cognitive and motor training on HRV indices. Physical training alone failed to impact HRV in older adults whereas dual cognitive and motor training significantly improved global and parasympathetic autonomic nervous system activity (). These studies point toward a duality; the vagal communications between heart and mind can be bidirectionally manipulated to improve both parasympathetic control of HRV and, synergistically, executive cognitive function.

Preclinical research has noted that VNS, particularly to the right vagus nerve, increases vagally mediated (vm-) HRV measures (). In a canine study, VNS treatment enhanced HRV at 4 and 8 weeks and reduced heart failure development () and a Japanese study in rabbits founds that intermittent VNS, but not constant VNS, increased the HF (vagal) component of HRV (). Discrepancies in this preclinical work may be due to different species, devices and parameters but indicate that manipulating the vagus nerve electrically can have positive impacts on cardiac function and HRV.

Vagus Nerve Stimulation and Heart Rate Variability in Healthy Volunteers

Transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation devices and their stimulation effect on HRV have been examined in several experimental paradigms involving multiple auricular positions, left vs. right ear stimulation, and different stimulation settings. There is a trend toward positive findings, i.e., improved HRV indices, with t-VNS in healthy volunteer populations when the right auricular branch of the vagus is stimulated (). It is notable that greater responses to t-VNS (i.e., improved vagally medicated HRV signals) have been demonstrated in those with higher sympathetic balance at baseline in both younger and older volunteers, both acutely and with 2 weeks t-VNS at home for 15 min daily (). An experimental design comparing left and right t-VNS at multiple stimulation targets found that SDNN and RMSSD both were most significantly improved when the right cymba conchae and fossa triangularis were stimulated ().

When specific parameters of stimulation at the left tragus were sequentially analyzed, the settings that had the most significant impact on heart rate analysis in young volunteers were 500 μs at 10 Hz (). Studies investigating the effect of t-VNS and 70-degree tilt table testing on HRV at the left tragus found that the RSA measure of HRV (HF domain) was also significantly increased during an orthostatic maneuver () and similarly stimulation at the left cymba conchae during 75-degree tilt found that responsivity, i.e., degree of change of heart rate and systolic blood pressure during t-VNS were significantly higher during orthostasis compared to control ().

Research in this area has not been consistent. Some initial findings indicated improved HRV measures with t-VNS to the left cymba conchae but ultimately no difference compared to sham and at multiple intensities (). In an experimental crossover design employing a variety of amplitudes at the right cymba, there was no positive signal in affecting HRV measures () and similarly t-VNS to the right tragus during rest and autonomic nervous system testing, with appreciably different stimulation parameters to what was previously cited in the literature, also did not have any effect on HRV (). Inconsistent results are likely due to the use of different anatomical sites and stimulation parameters being utilized, some with “lead in” times and some without, and reporting on this area has been of variable quality, and recent international consensus has called for standardized reporting of this research ().

Vagus Nerve Stimulation and Heart Rate Variability in Clinical Populations

Initial studies in clinical populations involved patients with iVNS devices inserted for control of refractory epilepsy. The earliest study demonstrated a reduction in LF:HF ratio and significantly higher HF power was noted in the higher stimulation group than lower stimulation (see Table 4). These results were not however replicated in further studies of similar populations with comparable stimulation settings at timeframes ranging from minutes to 1 year of stimulation (). A small study analyzing HRV in patients with iVNS devices implanted for management of treatment-resistant depression noted an increase in the RMSSD (increased vagal predominance) during stimulation compared to baseline and healthy controls (). It is notable that iVNS devices are for the most part inserted to activate the vagus via its left cervical branch, thereby appropriately reducing adverse cardiac effects but also not demonstrably influencing HRV measures in these populations.

TABLE 4.

VNS and neurocardiovascular assessment.

Neurocardiovascular assessment AND VNS
VNS Stimulation Parameters
StudyiVNS/tVNS/site specificHzmAPulse widthTimeAnalysis parametersPopulationResult
iVNS for refractory epilepsy (left cervical vagus)2 Hz
30 Hz
0.1 mA
1 mA
130 ms
500 ms
Not specifiedBaseline 45 min ECG readings pre implantation and at 2/52 post implantRefractory epilepsy n = 8
High stimulation and low stimulation groups avg age 34 ± 7.8 range 21–47
HiStim group: LF:HF ratio decreased from 2.5 ± 1.5 preimplant to 1.5 ± 0.49 (P < 0.02) with iVNS
Significantly higher HF power in the HiStim compared to LoStim group
iVNS for refractory epilepsy (left cervical vagus) implanted for minimum 1/1230 HzMax tolerated threshold750 μs30 s on 5 min offPre and post stimulation ECG (7 min baseline, 2.5 min of stimulation and a 7 min post-stimulation)Refractory epilepsy n = 10 (avg age 28 range 14–46) 8 menNo significant effect noted on HRV variables
iVNS for refractory epilepsy (left cervical vagus)30Hz in high stimulation group
I Hz in low stimulation group
Avg 1.3 mA in high simulation group
Avg 1.2 mA in low stimulation group
500 μs
130 μs
30 s on every 5 min
30 s on every 3 h
Study mainly aimed at seizure reduction in two groups (high vs. low stimulation) in refractory epilepsyRefractory epilepsy
High stimulation group
n = 95 age 32.1 ± 10.8
Low stimulation n = 103 age 34.2 ± 10.1
“Autonomic function assessments revealed no significant changes in Holter function measures; mean heart rate, mean lowest or highest heart rate, heart rate variability, occurrences of bradycardia”
iVNS for refractory epilepsy (left cervical vagus)30 Hz0.25 mA adjusted500 μs30 s on every 5 min24-h analysis of RR variability at baseline (t0), 1 month (t1, short-term VNS) and 36 months after VNS initiation (t2, long-term VNS).Refractory epilepsy n = 7 (4 men) age 47 ± 11.2 range 34–63 fNo significant changes in HRV variables, trend to increased HF at night-time
iVNS for refractory epilepsy (left cervical vagus)30 Hz2.9 mA avg500 ms30 s on 5 min offPre and 1 year post implantation 24 h Holter
HRV variables
Refractory epilepsy n = 14 (eight male and six female age 34.3 ± 9.3; 20–52) compared to matched controlsVNS had no significant effects on any HRV indices despite a significant reduction in seizure frequency
iVNS for refractory epilepsy (left cervical vagus)30 Hz0.75–1.75 mA500 μs30 s on, 5 s off24 h ECG holter at baseline and after 3/12 implantationRefractory epilepsy 8 patients (age 32 range 9–65 2 men)No significant change in HRV parameters after 3/12 iVNS
iVNS (left cervical vagus) for treatment resistant depression (post implantation 6–40 months)15–30Hz0.25–2.5 mA500 μs30 s on 5 min offECG testing at baseline, switched on and switched off conditionsPatients with major depressive disorder (ICD-10) n = 9 (51.6 years, 5 women, 4 men)
Compared to age and sex matched controls
RMSSD increased significantly in switched on conditions during stimulation (30 s) in six patients compared to stimulation-free intervals and baseline
tVNS on inner and outer surface of the tragus of the ear
Sham – on tragus but disconnected
Either active or sham tVNS
30Hz10–50 mA200 μsContinuous 15 min stimulationHRV frequency and spectral analysis
Muscle sympathetic nerve activity (MSNA) recordings
Healthy volunteers n = 48 age 20–62 years old (M:F 1:1)Significant decrease
in LF/HF ratio during active tVNS
Greater response to tVNS in those who had higher sympathetic predominance at baseline (higher LF/HF ratio)

Study 1
tVNS cymba conchae left or right ear vs. sham (earlobe)25Hz0.7 mA average250 μs30 s on/30 s off
10 min
HRV frequency and spectral analysisHealthy older volunteer n = 30 age 23–58Right stimulation alone significantly increased SDNN compared to baseline

Study 2
tVNS cymba conchae right ear25Hz1 mA average250 μs30 s on/30 s off
1 h
HRV frequency and spectral analysisHealthy older volunteer n = 30 age range 30–65SDNN significantly
increased after 35 min and after 1 h specifically in female participants
LF and LF/HF
significantly increased after 35 min of stimulation
tVNS
active – tragus- inner and outer surface
sham ear lobe
Electrodes placed bilaterally
(1) active tVNS
(2) sham- olunteer placed on tragus –no current
(3) olunteer placed on the earlobe current applied
30 Hz45 ± 1 mA200 μsContinuous 15 minHRV, BP variability, cBRSHealthy young male olunteer
n = 13
age = 23 ± 1
Active tVNS acutely improved spontaneous
cBRS, olunteer LF/HF ratio and evoked slight decrease in HR
Nil change with two sham conditions
tVNS left tragus/auditory meatus or sham (no current)20Hz5.6 mA range 3–11.3 mA100 μsunavailablePostural HRV via Tilt Table Test
Startle Blink Paradigm
Military veterans with PTSD and mild TBI n = 12 or healthy control n = 10 age 30 ± 7Significantly increased RSA (HF HRV) in tilt during tVNS
Trend toward reduced reactivity (via electrodermal response monitoring) to startle

Study 1
tVNS to the inner side of the left tragus (anode in the ear canal, cathode on
the surface of the tragus) of the left ear for 9 different stimulation rounds
sham = left earlobe
crossover design
1Hz
10 Hz
25 Hz
At 100 μs: tragus 9.28 ± 2.56 mA earlobe 6.5 ± 1.83 mA
At 200 μs tragus
5.32 ± 1.60 mA earlobe 3.64 ± 1.26 mA
At 500 μs tragus 3.0 ± 0.93 mA earlobe
1.97 ± 0.70 mA
100 μs
200 μs
500 μs
Stimulation period (60s)
recovery period (180s)
Heart rate analysisHealthy young adult olunteer n = 15 (M:F 1:1) age 26.5 ± 4.9Active stimulation olunteer HR more
than control stimulation on with these parameters:
500 μs at 25 Hz
500 μs at 10 Hz

Study 2
tVNS to the inner side of the left tragus (anode in the ear canal, cathode on
the surface of the tragus) of the left ear for 10 stimulation rounds
sham = left earlobe
crossover design
10 Hz
25 Hz
tragus-
2.09 ± 0.97 mA earlobe 2.04 ± 0.82 mA
500 μsStimulation period (60s)
recovery period (90s)
Heart rate analysisHealthy young adult olunteer n = 20 (M:F 1:1)The parameters 500 ms at 10 Hz alone
induced a significant decrease in HR

Study 1
tVNS left tragus
1 week later sham (electrodes on tragus but no current)
30Hz2–4 mA200 μs15 minBaroreceptor sensitivity §Healthy participants aged ≥55 years
n = 14
Age 69.11 ± 1.52
Baseline LF/HF ratio power significantly predicted response to tVNS where higher resting LF/HF ratio was associated with greater olunteer during tVNS

Study 2
tVNS left tragus no sham30 Hz2–4 mA200 μs15 minBaroreceptor sensitivity, HRV frequency and spectral analysisHealthy participants aged ≥55 years
n = 51
Age 65.20 ± 0.79
Total power, mean RR interval, Δ RR, SDRR were significantly affected during tVNS
A higher LF/HF ratio predicted a greater decrease to tVNS

Study 3
tVNS left tragus daily at home for 15 min for 2 weeks30Hz2–4 mA200 μs15 min daily for 14 daysHRV frequency and spectral analysisHealthy participants aged ≥55 years
n = 29
Age 64.14 ± 0.89
RMSSD, pRR50, SD1 and nSD1, were significantly higher after 2 weeks tVNS
tVNS left cymba conchae
Cross-over design 2-day protocol, 1 day with tVNS and a control day, at least 24 h difference
25Hz1–6 mA adjusted to sensory threshold200 μs10 min supine stimulator on (rest tVNS on),
15 min orthostatic position with tVNS on (tilt tVNS on)
(1) ECG
(2) Respiration
(3) Non-invasive beat-to-beat arterial blood pressure
at rest and during a 75° tilt test
Healthy young olunteer n = 13 (5 males, 8 females) age 27 ± 4 yearsClinostasis: tVNS reduced HR, systolic BP variability and cardiac and peripheral sympathetic modulation
Responsivity of HR and BP to orthostatic stress during tVNS was significantly higher when compared to control

Study 1
tVNS to left cymba conchae25Hz0.5, 1, and 1.5 mA200–300 μs30 s on/off cycling
10 min stimulation
RMSSDHealthy young olunteer n = 61 (16 female) avg age 23.32Increase in RMSSD during stimulation compared to the resting phases for all mA settings

Study 2
tVNS to left cymba conchae25Hz1 mA
Compared to 1.78 mA ± 1.13
200–300 μs30 s on/off cycling
10 min stimulation
RMSSDHealthy young olunteer n = 62
(26 females avg age 24.77)
RMSSD values showed a significant overall increase during the stimulation phase none of the different stimulation conditions significantly differed from each other regarding RMSSD values

Study 3
tVNS to left cymba conchae vs. sham (earlobe)25 HzActive 2.5 mA ± 0.93)
Sham 2.76 mA ± 1.01
200–300 μs30 s on/off cycling
10 min stimulation each
RMSSDHealthy young volunteers n = 60 (31 females, age avg 23.62)No difference between active and sham stimulation
tVNS
(1) to cymba conchae no current
(2) to cymba conchae active during exhalation
(3) to cymba conchae active during inhalation
(4) sham to earlobe
25 Hz(1) 1.6 mA ± 2.3
(2) 1.7 mA ± 2.4
(3) 1.4 mA ± 1.1
450
ms pulse width duration of 1 s
32 min-Instantaneous HF-HRV index
-four 8-min duration fMRI scans
(1) passive
control
(2) active stimulation exhalation
(3) active stimulation inhalation
(4) active control
Healthy adult participants n = 16 (9 female, age 27.0 ± 6.6)Exhalation tVNS but not inhalation enhanced cardiovagal modulation, i.e., increased instantaneous HF hRV index
Exhalation found significantly signal at MRI site of LC/NTS
tVNS Cymba
Crossover design
5Hz
20Hz active
5Hz sham
1.5 ± 0. mA
1.2 ± 0. mA
5.5 ± 1. mA
0.2 ms10 min stimulation
10 min washout
Muscle sympathetic nerve activity (MSNA) recorded by microneurography at rest, during apnoea and tVNS
HRV power and spectral analysis
Healthy, young male volunteers n = 28 (age 27 ± 4)Acute right cymba tVNS did not induce any effects on HRV nor MSNA variables when compared to active control
tVNS right n = 7 left n = 6
-cymba conchae
-cavum conchae
-outer tragus
-inner tragus
-crus helicis
-fossa triangularis
25 Hz at a periodicity of 1 Hz0.2–2 mA
0.096–0.769 mA
0.05–0.4 mA
100 μs
260 μs
260 μs
90 s (i.e., 3 s × 30 s) at each stimulation site
144 parameter combinations
HRV power and spectral analysisHealthy adults n = 13 (age 24 ± 3, 8 female)Significant differences between right- and left-sided stimulation for the SDNN and RMSSD analysis only (increasing with right ear stimulation)
HRV increases were highest at cymba conchae and fossa triangularis, to a lesser extent to stimulation at the inner tragus
tVNS to right tragus during rest (60 min) and autonomic nervous system testing (15 min) (Valsalva, wet cold face, etc.)
sham = no stimulation, preceded stimulation
20HzAdjusted individually
to barely perceptible <150 μA
1 ms
rectangular pulse width
1 h resting tVNS vs. sham
15 min ANST vs. sham
Continuous cardiac measurements with impedance cardiography
Non-invasive arterial BP monitor
ECG for HRV analysis
Healthy male volunteers n = 15 (age 23 range 20–25)Indices of LV contractility, LV output, and LV work significantly decreased
SBP and TPR significantly increased
No difference HRV or ANST parameters

Please see Table 4 for further analysis of the specific neurocardiovascular assessments, specific t-VNS parameters and outcomes measures in discreet populations in this area.

Vagus Nerve Stimulation, Cognition and Heart Rate Variability

Heart rate variability can be conceptualized as a biomarker of parasympathetic modulation, and it is associated with a network of brain regions involved in autonomic nervous system regulation, known as the central autonomic network (). This network, which comprises prefrontal cortical (anterior cingulate, insula, orbitofrontal, and ventromedial cortices), limbic (central nucleus of the amygdala, hypothalamus), and brainstem regions, areas of the brain intimately involved in emotional regulation and executive functioning, leading to the proposal that vagally mediated HRV may index these aspects of prefrontal cortical function (). Higher HRV has been linked to better cognitive function in healthy adults including healthy older individuals () and a meta-analysis found a positive overall correlation (r = 0.09) between vagally mediated HRV indices and emotional regulation processes (including executive functioning, emotion regulation, and effortful or self-control) in mostly healthy participants across a number of age groups ().

Autonomic system dysfunction is common in patients with MCI, with studies suggesting MCI participants are 5.6 times more likely than controls to have autonomic dysfunction, specifically on assessment of HRV and cardiac reflexes (). A meta-analysis of MCI with dementia also found autonomic dysfunction, as defined by reduced HRV, was significantly associated with cognitive impairment (). Reduced HRV is associated with worse performance on tests of global cognitive function, more than cardiovascular risk factors ().

Recent meta-analyses of HRV in patients with neurodegenerative conditions including MCI, Alzheimer’s disease, Lewy Body dementia (DLB), vascular dementia, Parkinson’s disease and multiple sclerosis found a significant, moderate effect (r = 0.25) indicating that higher HRV was related to better cognitive and behavioral scores, which was not influenced by mean age or cognitive status (). These results were mirrored in a similar recent meta-analysis of patients with dementia compared to healthy controls, which found significantly lower resting HRV for parasympathetic function and total variability in those with dementia. On subgroup analysis then most striking differences, i.e., worse HRV analysis was found in those with MCI or DLB ().

Heart rate variability and CBF are linked via vagal afferents, and a meta-analysis revealed that HRV was significantly associated with regional cerebral blood flow in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (including anterior cingulate regions) and the amygdala (). In both younger and older adults scanned while at rest, higher HRV is associated with higher medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala functional connectivity (). The Neurovisceral Integration Model holds that HRV, executive cognitive function, and prefrontal neural function are integrally associated ().

In an interesting Swedish clinical trial in 2002, iVNS devices were implanted in a small group of patients with likely Alzheimer’s Dementia (AD) as defined by the criteria of the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association (NINCDS-ADRDA), with a view to assessing its impact on cognition via memory test scores. In the primary trial, 10 patients with average Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE) scores of 21 (range 16–24) had iVNS devices implanted and the median change in MMSE and Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale-cognitive subscale (ADAS-cog) scores among a battery of tests was assessed at 3 and 6 months, with improvements in both assessments noted in the majority (6 out of 10) of cases (). The follow up trial by the same research group involved 17 patients with likely AD, who had iVNS devices implanted and had outcomes measured and available at 1 year post implantation. At 1 year, 7 of 17 (41%) had improvement or no decline from baseline in ADAS-cog scores and 12 of 17 (70%) had improvement or no decline in MMSE scores. There was no change in noted in other outcomes including depressive symptoms (). There are a small number of trials registered investigating the therapeutic potential of t-VNS in older populations, both healthy and with cognitive impairment (for a recent review see ()) however there are no known published studies to date investigating t-VNS in populations with dementia or MCI, and the associated effect on HRV.

Summary

There is mounting evidence of the potential benefits of VNS in myriad disease states, with notable promise in the area of cognition. VNS shows promise as a neuromodulatory technique in cognitive decline and this may be via its ability to regulate both cardiac autonomic function and increase cerebral perfusion. Dementia is a multifactorial process and together with reduced cerebral perfusion is associated with neuroinflammation and altered synaptic plasticity, both of which may also be favorably modulated by VNS. It has been noted that perfusion to cortical and subcortical areas increases with VNS, specifically to areas that modulate executive function and attention, i.e., insular, orbitofrontal and prefrontal cortex. These areas are hypothesized by the neurovisceral integration model to be crucial areas in modulating the ANS (). Given that the LC-NE system is intimately involved in the therapeutic effects of VNS, and likely improves cognition via norepinephrine release and improved executive performance, it is notable that the earliest stages of pathological tau accumulation in Alzheimer’s disease are seen in the LC. Whether this small midbrain nucleus will prove to be pivotal in our understanding of how to modulate the vagus nerve and harness its benefits cognitively remains to be elucidated. VNS can now be delivered safely and non-invasively via t-VNS devices with equivalent neuromodulatory effects on brain imaging as invasive devices, which broadens its therapeutic applicability considerably, especially to an older population with cognitive complaints for whom device implantation may not be feasible. Globally, the need for effective therapies to both treat the cause and symptoms of cognitive decline are needed urgently as rates of dementia increase due to population expansion. Dedicated studies into the potential therapeutic effects of t-VNS in early cognitive decline and dementia are needed. Research to date has been limited by myriad issues, including studies on cognition in clinical populations with altered neuroanatomy, lack of standardization in device usage, parameter settings, frequency of use, duration of stimulation. Minimum reporting standards have recently been published to help ameliorate some of these issues. Further rigorous studies of the therapeutic benefit of VNS are required, especially in populations with autonomic instability and cognitive decline.

Author Contributions

HD did most of the research, writing, and editing of the article. Significant contributions were made by each author, specifically TD with manuscript reading, editing, and direction, SC with direction RE psychological assessments and plasticity, CF with neurocardiovascular assessments, ANS testing. PM and SK assisted significantly with overall editorial support and guidance. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Funding

This study is part of a research study generously funded by the Meath Foundation, Tallaght University Hospital.

References

  1. Agostini E., Chinnock J., de Daly M., Murray J. (1957). Functional and histological studies of the vagus nerve and its branches to the heart, lungs and abdominal viscera in the cat. J. Physiol. 135 182–205. 10.1113/JPHYSIOL.1957.SP005703 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alsop D., Detre J., Grossman M. (2000). Assessment of cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer’s disease by spin-labeled magnetic resonance imaging. Ann. Neurol. 47 93–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alsop D. C., Casement M., de Bazelaire C., Fong T., Press D. Z. (2008). Hippocampal hyperperfusion in Alzheimer’s disease. NeuroImage 42 1267–1274. 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2008.06.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Antonino D., Teixeira A. L., Maia-Lopes P. M., Souza M. C., Sabino-Carvalho J. L., Murray A. R., et al. (2017). Non-invasive vagus nerve stimulation acutely improves spontaneous cardiac baroreflex sensitivity in healthy young men: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. Brain Stimul. 10 875–881. 10.1016/j.brs.2017.05.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Asllani I., Habeck C., Scarmeas N., Borogovac A., Brown T. R., Stern Y. (2008). Multivariate and univariate analysis of continuous arterial spin labeling perfusion MRI in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. Off. J. Int. Soc. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 28 725–736. 10.1038/SJ.JCBFM.9600570 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Aston-Jones G., Cohen J. D. (2005). An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28 403–450. 10.1146/ANNUREV.NEURO.28.061604.135709 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bachmann J., Munzert J., Krüger B. (2018). Neural underpinnings of the perception of emotional states derived from biological human motion: a review of neuroimaging research. Front. Psychol. 9:1763. 10.3389/FPSYG.2018.01763 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baddeley A. (2010). Working memory. Curr. Biol. 20 R136–R140. 10.1016/J.CUB.2009.12.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Badran B. W., Dowdle L. T., Mithoefer O. J., LaBate N. T., Coatsworth J., Brown J. C., et al. (2018a). Neurophysiologic effects of transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation (taVNS) via electrical stimulation of the tragus: a concurrent taVNS/fMRI study and review. Brain Stimul. 11 492–500. 10.1016/J.BRS.2017.12.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Badran B. W., Mithoefer O. J., Summer C. E., LaBate N. T., Glusman C. E., Badran A. W., et al. (2018b). Short trains of transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation (taVNS) have parameter-specific effects on heart rate. Brain Stimul. Basic Transl. Clin. Res. Neuromodul. 11 699–708. 10.1016/J.BRS.2018.04.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bagneux V., Thomassin N., Gonthier C., Roulin J. L. (2013). Working memory in the processing of the iowa gambling task: an individual differences approach. PLoS One 8:e81498. 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0081498 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bailey P., Bremer F. (1938). A sensory cortical representation of the vagus nerve: with a note on the effects of low blood pressure on the cortical electrogram. J. Neurophysiol. 1 405–412. 10.1152/JN.1938.1.5.405 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Barone L., Colicchio G., Policicchio D., di Clemente F., di Monaco A., Meglio M., et al. (2007). Effect of vagal nerve stimulation on systemic inflammation and cardiac autonomic function in patients with refractory epilepsy. Neuroimmunomodulation 14 331–336. 10.1159/000127360 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bechara A., Martin E. M. (2004). Impaired decision making related to working memory deficits in individuals with substance addictions. Neuropsychology 18 152–162. 10.1037/0894-4105.18.1.152 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Beckstead R. M., Norgren R. (1979). An autoradiographic examination of the central distribution of the trigeminal, facial, glossopharyngeal, and vagal nerves in the monkey. J. Comparat. Neurol. 184 455–472. 10.1002/CNE.901840303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Beishon L., Evley R., Panerai R. B., Subramaniam H., Mukaetova-Ladinska E., Robinson T., et al. (2019). Effects of brain training on brain blood flow (The Cognition and Flow Study-CogFlowS): protocol for a feasibility randomised controlled trial of cognitive training in dementia. BMJ Open 9:27817. 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-027817 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Beishon L., Haunton V. J., Panerai R. B., Robinson T. G. (2017). Cerebral hemodynamics in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 59 369–385. 10.3233/JAD-170181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Benarroch E. E. (1993). The central autonomic network: functional organization, dysfunction, and perspective. Mayo Clin. Proc. 68 988–1001. 10.1016/S0025-6196(12)62272-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Berridge C. W., Waterhouse B. D. (2003). The locus coeruleus–noradrenergic system: modulation of behavioral state and state-dependent cognitive processes. Brain Res. Rev. 42 33–84. 10.1016/S0165-0173(03)00143-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Berthoud H. R., Neuhuber W. L. (2000). Functional and chemical anatomy of the afferent vagal system. Auton. Neurosci. 85, 1–17. 10.1016/S1566-0702(00)00215-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Beste C., Steenbergen L., Sellaro R., Grigoriadou S., Zhang R., Chmielewski W., et al. (2016). Effects of concomitant stimulation of the GABAergic and norepinephrine system on inhibitory control – a study using transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation. Brain Stimul. 9 811–818. 10.1016/J.BRS.2016.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Bohning D. E., Lomarev M. P., Denslow S., Nahas Z., Shastri A., George M. S. (2001). Feasibility of vagus nerve stimulation-synchronized blood oxygenation level-dependent functional MRI. Invest. Radiol. 36 470–479. 10.1097/00004424-200108000-00006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Borges U., Knops L., Laborde S., Klatt S., Raab M. (2020). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation may enhance only specific aspects of the core executive functions. A randomized crossover trial. Front. Neurosci. 14:523. 10.3389/FNINS.2020.00523 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Borges U., Laborde S., Raab M. (2019). Influence of transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation on cardiac vagal activity: not different from sham stimulation and no effect of stimulation intensity. PLoS One 14:e0223848. 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0223848 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Bretherton B., Atkinson L., Murray A., Clancy J., Deuchars S., Deuchars J. (2019). Effects of transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation in individuals aged 55 years or above: potential benefits of daily stimulation. Aging 11 4836–4857. 10.18632/aging.102074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Broncel A., Bocian R., Kłos-Wojtczak P., Kulbat-Warycha K., Konopacki J. (2020). Vagal nerve stimulation as a promising tool in the improvement of cognitive disorders. Brain Res. Bull. 155 37–47. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2019.11.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Burger A. M., Van Diest I., Van der Does W., Korbee J. N., Waziri N., Brosschot J. F., et al. (2019). The effect of transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation on fear generalization and subsequent fear extinction. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 161 192–201. 10.1016/J.NLM.2019.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cakmak Y. O., Apaydin H., Kiziltan G., Gündüz A., Ozsoy B., Olcer S., et al. (2017). Rapid alleviation of parkinson’s disease symptoms via electrostimulation of intrinsic auricular muscle zones. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:338. 10.3389/FNHUM.2017.00338 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Camina E., Güell F. (2017). The neuroanatomical, neurophysiological and psychological basis of memory: current models and their origins. Front. Pharmacol. 8:438. 10.3389/FPHAR.2017.00438 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Chae J. H., Nahas Z., Lomarev M., Denslow S., Lorberbaum J. P., Bohning D. E., et al. (2003). A review of functional neuroimaging studies of vagus nerve stimulation (VNS). J. Psychiatric Res. 37 443–455. 10.1016/S0022-3956(03)00074-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Chalmers J. A., Quintana D. S., Abbott M. J. A., Kemp A. H. (2014). Anxiety disorders are associated with reduced heart rate variability: a meta-analysis. Front. Psychiatry 5:80. 10.3389/FPSYT.2014.00080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Chambers C. D., Garavan H., Bellgrove M. A. (2009). Insights into the neural basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33 631–646. 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2008.08.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Cheng Y. C., Huang Y. C., Huang W. L. (2022). Heart rate variability in patients with dementia or neurocognitive disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 56 16–27. 10.1177/0004867420976853 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Clancy J. A., Mary D. A., Witte K. K., Greenwood J. P., Deuchars S. A., Deuchars J. (2014). Non-invasive Vagus nerve stimulation in healthy humans reduces sympathetic nerve activity. Brain Stimul. 7 871–877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Clark K. B., Krahl S. E., Smith D. C., Jensen R. A. (1995). Post-training unilateral vagal stimulation enhances retention performance in the rat. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 63 213–216. 10.1006/NLME.1995.1024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Clark K. B., Naritoku D. K., Smith D. C., Browning R. A., Jensen R. A. (1999). Enhanced recognition memory following vagus nerve stimulation in human subjects. Nat. Neurosci. 2 94–98. 10.1038/4600 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Clewett D. V., Lee T. H., Greening S., Ponzio A., Margalit E., Mather M. (2016). Neuromelanin marks the spot: identifying a locus coeruleus biomarker of cognitive reserve in healthy aging. Neurobiol. Aging 37:117. 10.1016/J.NEUROBIOLAGING.2015.09.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Collins O., Dillon S., Finucane C., Lawlor B., Kenny R. A. (2012). Parasympathetic autonomic dysfunction is common in mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 33 2324–2333. 10.1016/J.NEUROBIOLAGING.2011.11.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Colzato L., Beste C. (2020). A literature review on the neurophysiological underpinnings and cognitive effects of transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation: challenges and future directions. J. Neurophysiol. 123 1739–1755. 10.1152/jn.00057.2020.-Brain [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Colzato L. S., Ritter S. M., Steenbergen L. (2018). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (tVNS) enhances divergent thinking. Neuropsychologia 111 72–76. 10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2018.01.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Colzato L. S., Sellaro R., Beste C. (2017). Darwin revisited: the vagus nerve is a causal element in controlling recognition of other’s emotions. Cortex 92 95–102. 10.1016/J.CORTEX.2017.03.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Conway C. R., Sheline Y. I., Chibnall J. T., Bucholz R. D., Price J. L., Gangwani S., et al. (2012). Brain blood-flow change with acute vagus nerve stimulation in treatment-refractory major depressive disorder. Brain Stimul. 5 163–171. 10.1016/J.BRS.2011.03.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Coote J. H. (2013). Myths and realities of the cardiac vagus. J. Physiol. 591 4073–4085. 10.1113/JPHYSIOL.2013.257758 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Cummings J., Lee G., Zhong K., Fonseca J., Taghva K. (2021). Alzheimer’s disease drug development pipeline: 2021. Alzheimer’s Dementia Transl. Res. Clin. Intervent. 7:e12179. 10.1002/TRC2.12179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. da Silva V. P., Ramalho Oliveira B. R., Tavares Mello R. G., Moraes H., Deslandes A. C., Laks J. (2017). Heart rate variability indexes in dementia: a systematic review with a quantitative analysis. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 15 80–88. 10.2174/1567205014666170531082352 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. de Couck M., Cserjesi R., Caers R., Zijlstra W. P., Widjaja D., Wolf N., et al. (2017). Effects of short and prolonged transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation on heart rate variability in healthy subjects. Autonomic Neurosci. Basic Clin. 203 88–96. 10.1016/j.autneu.2016.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. de Couck M., Mravec B., Gidron Y. (2012). You may need the vagus nerve to understand pathophysiology and to treat diseases. Clin. Sci. 122 323–328. 10.1042/CS20110299 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. de la Vega A., Brown M. S., Snyder H. R., Singel D., Munakata Y., Banich M. T. (2014). Individual differences in the balance of GABA to glutamate in pFC predict the ability to select among competing options. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26 2490–2502. 10.1162/JOCN_A_00655 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Ding B., Ling H. W., Zhang Y., Huang J., Zhang H., Wang T., et al. (2014). Pattern of cerebral hyperperfusion in Alzheimer’s disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment using voxel-based analysis of 3D arterial spin-labeling imaging: initial experience. Clin. Intervent. Aging 9 493–500. 10.2147/CIA.S58879 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Dixon M. L. (2015). Cognitive control, emotional value, and the lateral prefrontal cortex. Front. Psychol. 6:758. 10.3389/FPSYG.2015.00758 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Dodrill C. B., Morris G. L. (2001). Effects of vagal nerve stimulation on cognition and quality of life in epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2 46–53. 10.1006/EBEH.2000.0148 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Duann J. R., Ide J. S., Luo X., Li C. S. R. (2009). Functional connectivity delineates distinct roles of the inferior frontal cortex and presupplementary motor area in stop signal inhibition. J. Neurosci. 29 10171–10179. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1300-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Eggenberger P., Annaheim S., Kündig K. A., Rossi R. M., Münzer T., de Bruin E. D. (2020). Heart rate variability mainly relates to cognitive executive functions and improves through exergame training in older adults: a secondary analysis of a 6-month randomized controlled trial. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12:197. 10.3389/FNAGI.2020.00197 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Entschladen F., Drell T. L., Lang K., Joseph J., Zaenker K. S. (2004). Tumour-cell migration, invasion, and metastasis: navigation by neurotransmitters. Lancet Oncol. 5 254–258. 10.1016/S1470-2045(04)01431-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Eriksen C. W. (1995). The flankers task and response competition: a useful tool for investigating a variety of cognitive problems. Visual Cogn. 2 101–118. 10.1080/13506289508401726 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. Erlanger J., Gasser H. (1937). Electrical signs of nervous activity. Am. J. Psychiatry 93 1472–a–1473. 10.1176/AJP.93.6.1472-A [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  57. Farmer A. D., Strzelczyk A., Finisguerra A., Gourine A. V., Gharabaghi A., Hasan A., et al. (2021). International consensus based review and recommendations for minimum reporting standards in research on transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (Version 2020). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:568051. 10.3389/FNHUM.2020.568051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Fischer R., Ventura-Bort C., Hamm A., Weymar M. (2018). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (tVNS) enhances conflict-triggered adjustment of cognitive control. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 18 680–693. 10.3758/s13415-018-0596-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Foley J., Dubois F. S. (1937). Quantitative studies of the vagus nerve in the cat. I. The ratio of sensory to motor fibres. J. Comp. Neurol. 67 49–64. [Google Scholar]
  60. Forte G., Favieri F., Casagrande M. (2019). Heart rate variability and cognitive function: a systematic review. Front. Neurosci. 13:710. 10.3389/FNINS.2019.00710 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Frangos E., Ellrich J., Komisaruk B. R. (2015). Non-invasive access to the vagus nerve central projections via electrical stimulation of the external ear: fMRI evidence in humans. Brain Stimul. 8 624–636. 10.1016/J.BRS.2014.11.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Frangos E., Komisaruk B. R. (2017). Access to vagal projections via cutaneous electrical stimulation of the neck: fMRI evidence in healthy humans. Brain Stimul. 10 19–27. 10.1016/J.BRS.2016.10.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Frewen J., Finucane C., Savva G. M., Boyle G., Coen R. F., Kenny R. A. (2013). Cognitive function is associated with impaired heart rate variability in ageing adults: the Irish longitudinal study on ageing wave one results. Clin. Autonomic Res. 23 313–323. 10.1007/S10286-013-0214-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Furlan R., Heusser K., Minonzio M., Shiffer D., Cairo B., Tank J., et al. (2019). Cardiac and vascular sympathetic baroreflex control during orthostatic pre-syncope. J. Clin. Med. 8:1434. 10.3390/JCM8091434 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Galli R., Limbruno U., Pizzanelli C., Giorgi F. S., Lutzemberger L., Strata G., et al. (2003). Analysis of RR variability in drug-resistant epilepsy patients chronically treated with vagus nerve stimulation. Autonomic Neurosci. Basic Clin. 107 52–59. 10.1016/S1566-0702(03)00081-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Gao F., Edden R. A. E., Li M., Puts N. A. J., Wang G., Liu C., et al. (2013). Edited magnetic resonance spectroscopy detects an age-related decline in brain GABA levels. Neuroimage 78 75–82. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Gatti P. J., Johnson T. A., Massari V. J. (1996). Can neurons in the nucleus ambiguus selectively regulate cardiac rate and atrio-ventricular conduction? J. Autonomic Nerv. Syst. 57 123–127. 10.1016/0165-1838(95)00104-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Gauthey A., Morra S., van de Borne P., Deriaz D., Maes N., le Polain de Waroux J. B. (2020). Sympathetic effect of auricular transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation on healthy subjects: a crossover controlled clinical trial comparing vagally mediated and active control stimulation using microneurography. Front. Physiol. 11:599896. 10.3389/FPHYS.2020.599896 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Ghacibeh G. A., Shenker J. I., Shenal B., Uthman B. M., Heilman K. M. (2006a). Effect of vagus nerve stimulation on creativity and cognitive flexibility. Epilepsy Behav. 8 720–725. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2006.03.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Ghacibeh G. A., Shenker J. I., Shenal B., Uthman B. M., Heilman K. M. (2006b). The influence of vagus nerve stimulation on memory. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. Off. J. Soc. Behav. Cogn. Neurol. 19 119–122. 10.1097/01.WNN.0000213908.34278.7D [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Giraudier M., Ventura-Bort C., Weymar M. (2020). Transcutaneous Vagus Nerve Stimulation (tVNS) improves high-confidence recognition memory but not emotional word processing. Front. Psychol. 11:1276. 10.3389/FPSYG.2020.01276 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Goadsby P. J., Grosberg B. M., Mauskop A., Cady R., Simmons K. A. (2014). Effect of noninvasive vagus nerve stimulation on acute migraine: an open-label pilot study. Cephalalgia 34 986–993. 10.1177/0333102414524494 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Gonthier C. (2014). Cognitive Control in Working Memory: an Individual Differences Approach Based on the Dual Mechanisms of Control Framework. Saint-Martin-d’Hères: University of Grenoble. [Google Scholar]
  74. Grässler B., Hökelmann A., Cabral R. H. (2020). Resting heart rate variability as a possibly marker of cognitive decline. Kinesiology 52 72–84. 10.26582/k.52.1.9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  75. Groves D. A., Bowman E. M., Brown V. J. (2005). Recordings from the rat locus coeruleus during acute vagal nerve stimulation in the anaesthetised rat. Neurosci. Lett. 379 174–179. 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.12.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Haensel A., Mills P. J., Nelesen R. A., Ziegler M. G., Dimsdale J. E. (2008). The relationship between heart rate variability and inflammatory markers in cardiovascular diseases. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33 1305–1312. 10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2008.08.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Hämmerer D., Callaghan M. F., Hopkins A., Kosciessa J., Betts M., Cardenas-Blanco A., et al. (2018). Locus coeruleus integrity in old age is selectively related to memories linked with salient negative events. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115 2228–2233. 10.1073/PNAS.1712268115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Handforth A., DeGiorgio C. M., Schachter S. C., Uthman B. M., Naritoku D. K., Tecoma E. S., et al. (1998). Vagus nerve stimulation therapy for partial-onset seizures: a randomized active-control trial. Neurology 51 48–55. 10.1212/WNL.51.1.48 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Hansen A. L., Johnsen B. H., Sollers J. J., Stenvik K., Thayer J. F. (2004). Heart rate variability and its relation to prefrontal cognitive function: the effects of training and detraining. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 93 263–272. 10.1007/S00421-004-1208-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Hansen N. (2019). Memory reinforcement and attenuation by activating the human locus coeruleus via transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation. Front. Neurosci. 13:955. 10.3389/FNINS.2018.00955 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Hasan A., Wolff-Menzler C., Pfeiffer S., Falkai P., Weidinger E., Jobst A., et al. (2015). Transcutaneous noninvasive vagus nerve stimulation (tVNS) in the treatment of schizophrenia: a bicentric randomized controlled pilot study. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 265 589–600. 10.1007/S00406-015-0618-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Hays S. A., Rennaker R. L., Kilgard M. P. (2013). Targeting plasticity with vagus nerve stimulation to treat neurological disease. Progr. Brain Res. 207 275–299. 10.1016/B978-0-444-63327-9.00010-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Helmstaedter C., Hoppe C., Elger C. E. (2001). Memory alterations during acute high-intensity vagus nerve stimulation. Epilepsy Res. 47 37–42. 10.1016/S0920-1211(01)00291-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Henry T. R. (2002). Therapeutic mechanisms of vagus nerve stimulation. Neurology 59(Suppl. 4) S3–S14. 10.1212/WNL.59.6_SUPPL_4.S3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Henry T. R., Bakay R. A. E., Pennell P. B., Epstein C. M., Votaw J. R. (2004). Brain blood-flow alterations induced by therapeutic vagus nerve stimulation in partial epilepsy: II. Prolonged effects at high and low levels of stimulation. Epilepsia 45 1064–1070. 10.1111/J.0013-9580.2004.03104.X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Henry T. R., Bakay R. A. E., Votaw J. R., Pennell P. B., Epstein C. M., Faber T. L., et al. (1998). Brain blood flow alterations induced by therapeutic vagus nerve stimulation in partial epilepsy: I. Acute effects at high and low levels of stimulation. Epilepsia 39 983–990. 10.1111/J.1528-1157.1998.TB01448.X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Holzman J. B., Bridgett D. J. (2017). Heart rate variability indices as bio-markers of top-down self-regulatory mechanisms: a meta-analytic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 74 233–255. 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2016.12.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Huang F., Dong J., Kong J., Wang H., Meng H., Spaeth R. B., et al. (2014). Effect of transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation on impaired glucose tolerance: a pilot randomized study. BMC Complement. Alternat. Med. 14:203. 10.1186/1472-6882-14-203 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Huang J., Wang Y., Jiang D., Zhou J., Huang X. (2010). The sympathetic-vagal balance against endotoxemia. J. Neural Transm. 117 729–735. 10.1007/S00702-010-0407-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Hyvarinen P., Yrttiaho S., Lehtimaki J., Ilmoniemi R. J., Makitie A., Ylikoski J., et al. (2015). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation modulates tinnitus-related beta-and gamma-band activity. Ear Hear. 36 e76–e85. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000123 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Iwao T., Yonemochi H., Nakagawa M., Takahashi N., Saikawa T., Ito M. (2000). Effect of constant and intermittent vagal stimulation on the heart rate and heart rate variability in rabbits. Jpn. J. Physiol. 50 33–39. 10.2170/JJPHYSIOL.50.33 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Jacob G., Diedrich L., Sato K., Brychta R. J., Raj S. R., Robertson D., et al. (2019). Vagal and sympathetic function in neuropathic postural tachycardia syndrome. Hypertension 73 1087–1096. 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.118.11803/FORMAT/EPUB [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Jacobs H. I. L., Becker J. A., Kwong K., Engels-Domínguez N., Prokopiou P. C., Papp K. V., et al. (2021). In vivo and neuropathology data support locus coeruleus integrity as indicator of Alzheimer’s disease pathology and cognitive decline. Sci. Transl. Med. 13:eabj2511. 10.1126/scitranslmed.abj2511 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Jacobs H. I. L., Riphagen J. M., Razat C. M., Wiese S., Sack A. T. (2015). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation boosts associative memory in older individuals. Neurobiol. Aging 36 1860–1867. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.02.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Johnson N. A., Jahng G. H., Weiner M. W., Miller B. L., Chui H. C., Jagust W. J., et al. (2005). Pattern of cerebral hypoperfusion in Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment measured with arterial spin-labeling MR imaging: initial experience. Radiology 234 851–859. 10.1148/RADIOL.2343040197 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Jongkees B. J., Immink M. A., Finisguerra A., Colzato L. S. (2018). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (tVNS) Enhances response selection during sequential action. Front. Psychol. 9:1159. 10.3389/FPSYG.2018.01159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Kaan E., De Aguiar I., Clarke C., Lamb D. G., Williamson J. B., Porges E. C. (2021). A transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation study on verbal order memory. J. Neurolinguist. 59:100990. 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2021.100990 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  98. Kalia M., Sullivan J. M. (1982). Brainstem projections of sensory and motor components of the vagus nerve in the rat. J. Comparat. Neurol. 211 248–264. 10.1002/CNE.902110304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Kamath M., Upton A., Talalla A., Fallen E. (1992). Neurocardiac responses to vagoafferent electrostimulation in humans. Pacing Clin. Electrophysiol. PACE 15(Pt 2) 1581–1587. 10.1111/J.1540-8159.1992.TB02937.X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Kaniusas E., Kampusch S., Tittgemeyer M., Panetsos F., Gines R. F., Papa M., et al. (2019). Current directions in the auricular vagus nerve stimulation II - An engineering perspective. Front. Neurosci. 13:772. 10.3389/FNINS.2019.00772 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Kealy J., Commins S. (2010). Frequency-dependent changes in synaptic plasticity and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) expression in the CA1 to perirhinal cortex projection. Brain Res. 1326, 51–61. 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.02.065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Keute M., Barth D., Liebrand M., Heinze H.-J., Kraemer U., Zaehle T. (2020). Effects of Transcutaneous Vagus Nerve Stimulation (tVNS) on conflict-related behavioral performance and frontal midline theta activity. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 4 121–130. 10.1007/S41465-019-00152-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  103. Ko D., Heck C., Grafton S., Apuzzo M. L. J., Couldwell W. T., Chen T., et al. (1996). Vagus nerve stimulation activates central nervous system structures in epileptic patients during PET H215O blood flow imaging. Neurosurgery 39 426–431. 10.1097/00006123-199608000-00061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Koenig J., Parzer P., Haigis N., Liebemann J., Jung T., Resch F., et al. (2021). Effects of acute transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation on emotion recognition in adolescent depression. Psychol. Med. 51 511–520. 10.1017/S0033291719003490 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Koizumi K., Terui N., Kollai M. (1983). Neural control of the heart: significance of double innervation re-examined. J. Autonomic Nerv. Syst. 7 279–294. 10.1016/0165-1838(83)90081-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Kornblum S., Hasbroucq T., Osman A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility-a model and taxonomy. Psychol. Rev. 97 253–270. 10.1037/0033-295X.97.2.253 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Kosel M., Brockmann H., Frick C., Zobel A., Schlaepfer T. E. (2011). Chronic vagus nerve stimulation for treatment-resistant depression increases regional cerebral blood flow in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 191 153–159. 10.1016/J.PSCYCHRESNS.2010.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Kraus T., Hösl K., Kiess O., Schanze A., Kornhuber J., Forster C. (2007). BOLD fMRI deactivation of limbic and temporal brain structures and mood enhancing effect by transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation. J. Neural Transm. 114 1485–1493. 10.1007/S00702-007-0755-Z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Kraus T., Kiess O., Hösl K., Terekhin P., Kornhuber J., Forster C. (2013). CNS BOLD fMRI effects of sham-controlled transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation in the left outer auditory canal - A pilot study. Brain Stimul. 6 798–804. 10.1016/j.brs.2013.01.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Kunii N., Koizumi T., Kawai K., Shimada S., Saito N. (2021). Vagus nerve stimulation amplifies task-induced cerebral blood flow increase. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:726087. 10.3389/FNHUM.2021.726087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Lam J., Williams M., Ashla M., Lee D. J. (2021). Cognitive outcomes following vagus nerve stimulation, responsive neurostimulation and deep brain stimulation for epilepsy: a systematic review. Epilepsy Res. 172:106591. 10.1016/J.EPLEPSYRES.2021.106591 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Lamb D. G., Porges E. C., Lewis G. F., Williamson J. B. (2017). Non-invasive vagal nerve stimulation effects on hyperarousal and autonomic state in patients with Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and history of mild traumatic brain injury: preliminary evidence. Front. Med. 4:124. 10.3389/FMED.2017.00124 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Lanska D. J. (2002). J.L. Corning and vagal nerve stimulation for seizures in the 1880s. Neurology 58 452–459. 10.1212/WNL.58.3.452 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Laqua R., Leutzow B., Wendt M., Usichenko T. (2014). Transcutaneous vagal nerve stimulation may elicit anti- and pro-nociceptive effects under experimentally-induced pain - a crossover placebo-controlled investigation. Autonomic Neurosci. Basic Clin. 185 120–122. 10.1016/J.AUTNEU.2014.07.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Liu D., Hu Y. (1988). The central projections of the great auricular nerve primary afferent fibers - an HRP transganglionic tracing method. Brain Res. 445 205–210. 10.1016/0006-8993(88)91179-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Liu K. Y., Elliott T., Knowles M., Howard R. (2022). Heart rate variability in relation to cognition and behavior in neurodegenerative diseases: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res. Rev. 73:101539. 10.1016/J.ARR.2021.101539 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Liu W. C., Mosier K., Kalnin A. J., Marks D. (2003). BOLD fMRI activation induced by vagus nerve stimulation in seizure patients. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 74 811–813. 10.1136/JNNP.74.6.811 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Llewellyn-Smith I. J., Verberne A. J. M. (2011). Central Regulation of Autonomic Functions. Oxford: Oxford University, 1–400. [Google Scholar]
  119. Lomarev M., Denslow S., Nahas Z., Chae J. H., George M. S., Bohning D. E. (2002). Vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) synchronized BOLD fMRI suggests that VNS in depressed adults has frequency/dose dependent effects. J. Psychiatric Res. 36 219–227. 10.1016/S0022-3956(02)00013-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Machetanz K., Berelidze L., Guggenberger R., Gharabaghi A. (2021). Transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation and heart rate variability: analysis of parameters and targets. Autonomic Neurosci. Basic Clin. 236:102894. 10.1016/j.autneu.2021.102894 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. MacLeod C. M. (1991). Half a century of reseach on the stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 109 163–203. 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Maharjan A., Wang E., Peng M., Cakmak Y. O. (2018). Improvement of olfactory function with high frequency non-invasive auricular electrostimulation in healthy humans. Front. Neurosci. 12:225. 10.3389/FNINS.2018.00225 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Martin C. O., Denburg N. L., Tranel D., Granner M. A., Bechara A. (2004). The effects of vagus nerve stimulation on decision-making. Cortex 40 605–612. 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70156-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Massari V. J., Johnson T. A., Gatti P. J. (1995). Cardiotopic organization of the nucleus ambiguus? An anatomical and physiological analysis of neurons regulating atrioventricular conduction. Brain Res. 679 227–240. 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00227-H [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Mazza M., Marano G., Traversi G., Bria P., Mazza S. (2011). Primary cerebral blood flow deficiency and Alzheimer’s disease: shadows and lights. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 23 375–389. 10.3233/JAD-2010-090700 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. McGlone J., Valdivia I., Penner M., Williams J., Sadler R. M., Clarke D. B. (2008). Quality of life and memory after vagus nerve stimulator implantation for epilepsy. Can. J. Neurol. Sci. J. Can. Sci. Neurol. 35 287–296. 10.1017/S0317167100008854 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. McIntire L. K., McKinley R. A., Goodyear C., McIntire J. P., Brown R. D. (2021). Cervical transcutaneous vagal nerve stimulation (ctVNS) improves human cognitive performance under sleep deprivation stress. Commun. Biol. 4:634. 10.1038/s42003-021-02145-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Mercante B., Deriu F., Rangon C.-M. (2018). Auricular neuromodulation: the emerging concept beyond the stimulation of vagus and trigeminal nerves. Medicines 5:10. 10.3390/medicines5010010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Merrick J., Grippo A. J., Bartlett G., Shaffer F., Ginsberg J. P. (2017). An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Front. Public Health 5:258. 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Merrill C. A., Jonsson M. A. G., Minthon L., Ejnell H., Silander H. C. S., Blennow K., et al. (2006). Vagus nerve stimulation in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: additional follow-up results of a pilot study through 1 year. J. Clin. Psychiatry 67 1171–1178. 10.4088/JCP.V67N0801 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Mertens A., Naert L., Miatton M., Poppa T., Carrette E., Gadeyne S., et al. (2020). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation does not affect verbal memory performance in healthy volunteers. Front. Psychol. 11:551. 10.3389/FPSYG.2020.00551 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Montano N., Porta A., Cogliati C., Costantino G., Tobaldini E., Casali K. R., et al. (2009). Heart rate variability explored in the frequency domain: a tool to investigate the link between heart and behavior. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33 71–80. 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2008.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Morris G. L., Mueller W. M. (1999). Long-term treatment with vagus nerve stimulation in patients with refractory epilepsy. The vagus nerve stimulation study group E01-E05. Neurology 53 1731–1735. 10.1212/WNL.53.8.1731 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Mu Q., Bohning D. E., Nahas Z., Walker J., Anderson B., Johnson K. A., et al. (2004). Acute vagus nerve stimulation using different pulse widths produces varying brain effects. Biol. Psychiatry 55 816–825. 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2003.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Munakata Y., Herd S. A., Chatham C. H., Depue B. E., Banich M. T., O’Reilly R. C. (2011). A unified framework for inhibitory control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15:453. 10.1016/J.TICS.2011.07.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Narayanan J. T., Watts R., Haddad N., Labar D. R., Li P. M., Filippi C. G. (2002). Cerebral activation during vagus nerve stimulation: a functional MR study. Epilepsia 43 1509–1514. 10.1046/J.1528-1157.2002.16102.X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Naveh-Benjamin M. (2000). Adult age differences in memory performance: tests of an associative deficit hypothesis. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 26 1170–1187. 10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1170 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Neftel W. B. (1871). Galvano-Therapeutics. The Physiological and Therapeutical Action of the Galvanic Current Upon the Acoustic, Optic, Sympathetic, and Pneumogastric Nerves. New York, NY: D Appleton & Company. [Google Scholar]
  139. Nesbitt A. D., Marin J. C. A., Tompkins E., Ruttledge M. H., Goadsby P. J. (2015). Initial use of a novel noninvasive vagus nerve stimulator for cluster headache treatment. Neurology 84 1249–1253. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001394 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Nichols E., Steinmetz J. D., Vollset S. E., Fukutaki K., Chalek J., Abd-Allah F., et al. (2022). Estimation of the global prevalence of dementia in 2019 and forecasted prevalence in 2050: an analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet Public Health 7 e105–e125. 10.1016/S2468-2667(21)00249-8/ATTACHMENT/60E03FD1-38B2-4B40-A91D-9AFDDA22B45E/MMC1.PDF [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Obst M. A., Heldmann M., Alicart H., Tittgemeyer M., Münte T. F. (2020). Effect of Short-Term Transcutaneous Vagus Nerve Stimulation (tVNS) on brain processing of food cues: an electrophysiological study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:206. 10.3389/FNHUM.2020.00206 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Öngür D., Price J. L. (2000). The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb. Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991) 10 206–219. 10.1093/CERCOR/10.3.206 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. O’Reardon J. P., Cristancho P., Peshek A. D. (2006). Vagus Nerve Stimulation (VNS) and treatment of depression: to the brainstem and beyond. Psychiatry (Edgmont) 3:54. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Perini G. I., Toffanin T., Pigato G., Ferri G., Follador H., Zonta F., et al. (2017). Hippocampal gray volumes increase in treatment-resistant depression responding to vagus nerve stimulation. J. ECT 33 160–166. 10.1097/YCT.0000000000000424 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Petelin Gadze Z., Bujan Kovac A., Adamec I., Milekic N., Sulentic V. (2018). Vagal nerve stimulation is beneficial in postural orthostatic tachycardia syndrome and epilepsy. Seizure 57 11–13. 10.1016/J.SEIZURE.2018.03.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Peuker E. T., Filler T. J. (2002). The nerve supply of the human auricle. Clin. Anatomy 15 35–37. 10.1002/ca.1089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Pickering T. G., Davies J. (1973). Estimation of the conduction time of the baroreceptor-cardiac reflex in man. Cardiovasc. Res. 7 213–219. 10.1093/CVR/7.2.213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Polak T., Markulin F., Ehlis A. C., Langer J. B. M., Ringel T. M., Fallgatter A. J. (2009). Far field potentials from brain stem after transcutaneous Vagus nerve stimulation: optimization of stimulation and recording parameters. J. Neural Transm. 116 1237–1242. 10.1007/S00702-009-0282-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Porges E. C., Woods A. J., Edden R. A. E., Puts N. A. J., Harris A. D., Chen H., et al. (2017). Frontal gamma-aminobutyric acid concentrations are associated with cognitive performance in older adults. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 2:38. 10.1016/J.BPSC.2016.06.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Ridgewell C., Heaton K. J., Hildebrandt A., Couse J., Leeder T., Neumeier W. H. (2021). The effects of transcutaneous auricular vagal nerve stimulation on cognition in healthy individuals: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychology 35 352–365. 10.1037/NEU0000735 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Roges R. C., Kita H., Butcher L. L., Novin D. (1980). Afferent projections to the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus. Brain Res. Bull. 5 365–373. 10.1016/S0361-9230(80)80006-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Ronkainen E., Korpelainen J. T., Heikkinen E., Myllylä V. V., Huikuri H. V., Isojärvi J. I. T. (2006). Cardiac autonomic control in patients with refractory epilepsy before and during vagus nerve stimulation treatment: a one-year follow-up study. Epilepsia 47 556–562. 10.1111/J.1528-1167.2006.00467.X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Rufener K. S., Geyer U., Janitzky K., Heinze H. J., Zaehle T. (2018). Modulating auditory selective attention by non-invasive brain stimulation: differential effects of transcutaneous vagal nerve stimulation and transcranial random noise stimulation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 48 2301–2309. 10.1111/ejn.14128 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Rutecki P. (1990). Anatomical, physiological, and theoretical basis for the antiepileptic effect of vagus nerve stimulation. Epilepsia 31(Suppl. 2) S1–S6. 10.1111/J.1528-1157.1990.TB05843.X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Sakaki M., Yoo H. J., Nga L., Lee T.-H., Thayer J. F., Mather M. (2016). Heart rate variability is associated with amygdala functional connectivity with MPFC across younger and older adults HHS 

Comments

Popular Posts